Last Sunday, I posted an omnium
gatherum that included a response to Carl's friend, who had commented to
Carl about my post on The Origin of
Science by Louis Liebenberg. This
morning Carl posted a follow-up comment, also from his friend. [I find the social structure of the Internet
a trifle odd, but then I am very old.] I
am afraid I have brought on this exchange by the unclarity of my original
report of Liebenberg's book, which has an extremely misleading title. Since I find Liebenberg's central point
fascinating, I am going to try again to explain it. It has, let me say right up front, virtually nothing
to do with the origin of modern science, so far as I can tell [hence the inappropriateness
of the title.]
Some long time ago -- maybe half a million years, maybe only
two or three hundred thousand years, I am not sure -- animals of the genus homo ranging across the East African
savanna developed really big brains. These
brains were [and are] way larger than one would expect in mammals of their
size. The random mutations that produced
these enlarged brains must have had significant survival value in order to take
hold and become characteristic of late hominids, including of course homo sapiens sapiens, which is to say us. Leibenberg observes that these big brains are
required for scientific reasoning [and also for writing iambic pentameter,
although Liebenberg does not seem to notice that], but since what we identify
as advanced human culture came along way after the brains got big, that fact
can have played no part in the evolution of the big brains, teleological
explanations being definitely unacceptable.
Now Liebenberg is pretty clearly a novice on the
subject of the history and philosophy of
science, but he is a world-class expert on the people who practice a
hunter-gatherer existence in the Kalahari desert today -- people sometimes
referred to as the Zhu. When the Zhu
hunt, they engage in what is called persistence tracking. They do not lie in wait and attack animals,
or charge them and shoot them with bows and arrows. Instead they run an animal down, tracking it
for eight, ten, twelve hours or more in the great heat of the desert, until the
animal grows exhausted and simply lies down, at which point it can be killed.
Persistence tracking requires an enormous amount of very
detailed knowledge, not only of the general habits of each species of game, but
also of the most minute variations in the tracks they leave in sand or on hard
clay or in brush. Since the hunters are
pursuing one particular animal, trying to exhaust it, they must be able to
identify its tracks in the midst of many other overlapping tracks of the same
or other species.
Furthermore [and this is Liebenberg's big boffo point], it
often happens that the hunters lose the track -- the animal may run over rocky
terrain, for example. When this happens,
the very most skilled hunters [not all Zhu are alike in their hunting skills]
have the ability to form hypotheses about where the animal has gone, based both
on general knowledge and on their reading of subtle signs. They quite consciously identify with the
animal, asking themselves, "where would I go if I were that
kudu?" Liebenberg calls this
"speculative tracking." Clearly,
the ability to engage in successful speculative tracking will greatly improve
the chances of making a kill, and hence of being able to survive.
Thus far, I think Liebenberg has a good deal on his
side. Now he makes his big [and very
debatable] leap. The intellectual
capabilities called for by speculative tracking, he argues, which quite
plausibly appeared several hundred thousand years ago, are fundamentally the
same at some very basic and general level as the techniques of reasoning
employed by modern scientists. The big
brains were, so to speak, ready at hand when social, historical, economic and
other factors combined to produce the rise of modern science. In effect, he says, the early hominid hunter
gatherers engaging in speculative persistence tracking were proto-scientists.
If you begin with the established fact of the emergence of big-brained
hominids several hundred thousand years ago and ask what specific survival
skills those big brains made possible for those hominids, Liebenberg's
hypothesis has a certain plausibility.
Does any of that make sense?
No comments:
Post a Comment