Earlier today, I put up a snarky post about Donald Trump and
the Montréal Cognitive Assessment test that he bragged about acing. Now I
should like to return to the subject and this time write quite seriously about
a problem that insensitive doctors have in dealing with patients of my age. My
wife, Susan, is 87 years old. 30 years ago she was diagnosed with multiple
sclerosis and she has been dealing with the disease ever since. Fortunately,
the physical effects of the disease have been much less severe than in many
other cases and although she has a number of bodily discomforts that she
describes as “tingling and burning,” she still walks easily, usually with a
cane that she often does not need. However, in the past five or six years the
disease has inflicted upon her certain cognitive effects that make it more
difficult for her to remember things, to perform complex tasks of a certain
sort, and for example, to remember without my prompting to take her daily
medications.
When we moved to North Carolina we were quite fortunate to
get on the service of a first-rate doctor in the UNC health service and for a number
of years he looked after both of us, but perhaps four years ago or perhaps a
bit more he left to take some big deal job in Medicare and we had to transfer
to other primary care physicians. Susan was transferred to the practice of a
young, chirpy, cheerful, relentlessly friendly young woman who was obviously
accustomed to being loved by her patients. The first time Susan saw her, with
me in attendance as usual, as part of the examination she gave Susan a
shortened version of the cognitive assessment survey – clearly something that
is standard issue for all physicians in the UNC system. Susan is a proud woman
who for many years before we married supported herself and her sons as a real
estate agent. Susan graduated with a Phi Beta Kappa key from Connecticut
College for Women in 1954 and after moving to Chicago with her first husband,
taught botany to premed students at the University of Chicago and worked for 10
years in a research laboratory. She is extremely sensitive about the cognitive
deficits that the MS has inflicted upon her but, like the people of our
generation, she almost never says anything about this.
As the doctor started to administer the cognitive skills
test, Susan felt humiliated by her inability to do easily the simple tasks
required by the test and, as I could tell but the doctor could not, she froze
up, simply shaking her head as each new task was presented to her. The last
task of the test was to write a simple sentence and when the doctor read the
sentence that she had written she was startled and surprised. Susan had
written, “I am very unhappy.” “Good heavens, why?” The doctor burst out. I
could see that the doctor was accustomed to being adored by her patients and
could not understand what Susan could possibly be unhappy about. I knew that
she was not in fact unhappy, she was angry at having been humiliated, but
people of our age and generation don’t say things like that.
When we got home, I wrote a long letter to the doctor
explaining exactly what had happened and why Susan was so angry. The story has
a happy ending. We found a new doctor for Susan who is everything we could want
in a primary care physician, sensitive, understanding, supportive, and willing
to work with Susan to make her life more pleasant and manageable.
Now you might think that the moral of this story is that doctors
should be nicer people, and there is no doubt that that is true. But that is
not really the point of the story at all. The real point is this. The purpose
of administering the test to a new patient is to establish a baseline of
cognitive performance. When the test is administered again a year or two later,
the doctor can compare the score on the first test with the new score and
determine whether there has been a cognitive decline in the interim. But
because the doctor had administered the test insensitively and without any
awareness of the stress that it would produce in an elderly patient concerned
about her cognitive losses, the score recorded in the first administration of
the test was inaccurate. Susan was capable of doing much better at that time
than she actually did. At a later time, when the same test was administered by
a more thoughtful doctor, Susan might achieve exactly the same score. The new
doctor would think, “Good, there has been no decline in the interim.” In fact,
however, they might in the interim has been a genuine decline that the new
doctor missed. This is, of course, not a
problem with blood tests or an MRI. The results are what they are regardless of
the manner of the administering technician.
The UNC medical school has adopted the practice of having
their first year medical students meet in small groups with senior citizens
like myself for an informal conversation so that they can get to know the sorts
of people they may one day have as patients. I have several times signed up for
this session and each time I tell the new medical students this story in an
attempt to alert them to the special problems they will face in treating old
people.
Incidentally, before writing this post I asked Susan whether
she was comfortable with having me tell the story and she said that she was.
This effect is so common with patients with high blood pressure that it's called the "white coat hypertension".
ReplyDeletehttps://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/White_coat_hypertension
Anyone who's had a child with cognitive disabilities can relate to this type of thing. And it doesn't always have a happy ending.
ReplyDeleteHot takes from a young and dumb medical student: increasingly, medicine is more science and less philosophy, which is a problem when your job descriptions involve 'cluster properties' or whatever else Health and preferences are...and when you deal frequently with the genuine philosophical minefield of mental illness and congition. Many medical professionals just never seriously entertain thoughts about, for example, what it really MEANS to have a mental illness, or to what degree value is lost through dementia-family diseases, or even what the role of a doctor really is (maximising utility? preferences? being virtuous? This is best exemplified by Beauchamp and Childress's Principles of Medical Ethics, which is all the ethics medical people ever bother with, and its main thesis is that the disagreements between different ethical theories don't really matter anyway and can be efficiently abstracted over).
ReplyDelete